ABSTRACT Children’s assignment of novel words to nameless - TopicsExpress



          

ABSTRACT Children’s assignment of novel words to nameless objects, over objects whose names they know (mutual exclusivity; ME) has been described as a driving force for vocabulary acquisition. Despite their ability to use ME to fast-map words (Preissler & Carey, 2005), children with autism show impaired language acquisition. We aimed to address this puzzle by building on studies showing that correct referent selection using ME does not lead to word learning unless ostensive feedback is provided on the child’s object choice (Horst & Samuelson, 2008). We found that although toddlers aged 2;0 at risk for autism can use ME to choose the correct referent of a word, they do not benefit from feedback for long-term retention of the word–object mapping. Further, their difficulty using feedback is associated with their smaller receptive vocabularies. We propose that difficulties learning from social feedback, not lexical principles, limits vocabulary building during development in children at risk for autism. When studying how children learn words, we are faced with the famous Quinean puzzle – how do children know which of the many objects in their visual field is the referent of a word they hear (i.e. referent indeterminacy; Quine, 1960) ? Young language learners have many available strategies that help them to solve this correspondence problem throughout development. Earlier on, rich ostensive and referential cues are needed for children to acquire their first words; the caregiver or experimenter must ostensively direct the child’s attention to an object and repeatedly label it (Hollich et al., 2000; Houston-Price, Plunkett & Duffy, 2006; Woodward & Hoyne, 1999). Later in development, word learning has been demonstrated in the absence of such cues, in which case infants make use of various heuristics to infer a speaker’s referent. One such heuristic – the mutual exclusivity (ME) principle – refers to the assumption that novel words refer to unfamiliar objects or objects for which the child does not yet have a label. Most children start using this principle towards the end of their second year of life (Halberda, 2003; Markman & Wachtel, 1988). It is believed that children We are very grateful for the enormous contributions BASIS families have made towards this study. We thank Natasa Ganea for helping with data analysis. The research is supported by a Bloomsbury Colleges Scholarship to R. Bedford, the BASIS funding consortium led by Autistica (basisnetwork.org), Autism Speaks (PI M. H. Johnson, Grant number 1292) and a UK Medical Research Council Programme Grant (G0701484) to M. H. Johnson. The Centre for Research in Autism and Education is supported by The Clothworkers’ Foundation and Pears Foundation. T. Charman and M. Elsabbagh are supported by the COST Action BM1004. Address for correspondence: Rachael Bedford, Centre for Research in Autism and Education, Department of Psychology and Human Development, Institute of Education, 25 Woburn Square, London, WC1H 0AA, London, UK. e-mail: [email protected] BEDFORD ET AL. 30learn the ME principle by noticing that objects tend to be referred to using only one name (Markman, 1991). When exceptions occur, as in bilingual environments, children are less likely to treat object names as mutually exclusive (e.g. Davidson, Jergovic, Imami & Theodos, 1997). Because a word–object association appears to be possible after only one labelling episode (Carey & Bartlett, 1978), word learning in this context is referred to as FAST MAPPING, and has frequently been described as a driving force of the ‘vocabulary explosion’ seen at the end of the second year of life (e.g. Markman, Wasow & Hansen, 2003). However, more recently, a few studies have challenged the central role given to referent selection through ME for vocabulary growth (Horst & Samuelson, 2008; Mather & Plunkett, 2011). A typical ME task presents the children with two or more objects, one of which is unfamiliar. The child is asked to retrieve or to look at the dax (or another pseudo-word) (Halberda, 2003; Merriman & Schuster, 1991). Correct referent selection (either taking the novel object or looking longer at it) is in this case taken to reflect correct word learning. However, this differs from the testing of word learning and retention in ostensive word learning situations. In this latter case, looking at the referred object while it is labelled may only reflect cue following and is therefore not considered sufficient evidence. Correct word–object mapping is typically tested in a separate trial, following the labelling episode, with the child asked to choose the correct referent of a newly learned word amongst two previously labelled objects (Gliga, Elsabbagh, Hudry, Charman Johnson & the BASIS Team, in press; Houston-Price et al., 2006). When a similar procedure has been used to test word retention following fast mapping, results have been surprisingly negative. Horst and Samuelson (2008) showed that toddlers who were successful at using ME to choose the correct referent of a new word performed at chance when asked to retrieve that object 5 minutes later. Interestingly, the children’s performance in the retention trials improved if, after their initial correct choice, the experimenter reinforced their knowledge by ostensively labelling the object (i.e. by holding the object while pointing to it and naming it). These findings suggest that applying the ME principle may be necessary for quickly finding the referent of a new word but is not sufficient for that word to enter the child’s vocabulary. On the contrary, feedback upon the child’s initial choice seems to be crucial in creating a long-term word–object mapping. Apart from adding to our understanding of word learning mechanisms, these findings have the potential to clarify a contentious issue in language acquisition in autism. Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are less responsive to social cues, in particular to referential cues (Fletcher-Watson, Leekam, Benson, Frank & Findlay, 2009; Leekam & Ramsden, 2006), and experimental studies have shown that they have FEEDBACK REQUIRED FOR VOCABULARY LEARNING 31difficulties using such cues for word learning (Baron-Cohen, Baldwin & Crowson, 1997). However, referent selection through ME seems to be intact in this population (de Marchena, Eigsti, Worek, Ono & Snedeker, 2011; Preissler & Carey, 2005). This seems surprising given that children with autism have smaller vocabularies than expected for their age (Charman, Drew, Baird & Baird, 2003; Hudry et al., 2010; Tager-Flusberg, Paul & Lord, 2005), and further that delays in language acquisition (not accompanied by non-verbal gestural communication) form part of the diagnostic criteria for ASD (ICD-10; WHO, 1993). There are two possible explanations for this discrepancy. First, it could be that studies have overestimated these children’s referent selection abilities. Because autism is rarely diagnosed before two years of age, ME has mostly been assessed in older children (Preissler & Carey, 2005, tested five- to nine-year-olds; de Marchena et al., 2011, assessed children aged seven to eleven years). Their ability to use the ME constraint might have been the result of an extended learning process and therefore not a contributor to word learning earlier in development. Despite their age and proven ME skills, average comprehension vocabulary in Preissler and Carey’s (2005) sample was equivalent only to that of a typical two-year-old. A second explanation for the discrepancy between vocabulary size and word learning strategies may be explained by possible word retention difficulties. It could be that children with ASD, who are less sensitive to social–communicative cues, do not benefit from ostensive feedback and are therefore less able to retain the word–object mapping, despite their demonstrated ability to use the ME bias. To tease apart these hypotheses we replicated Horst and Samuelson’s (2008) study with a sample of two-year-olds who were either at high or low risk for ASD. Later-born siblings of children with ASD are at increased genetic risk of having an ASD themselves (henceforth high-risk children), relative to infants with no such family history (low-risk children) (Bolton et al., 1994; Constantino, Zhang, Frazier, Abbacchi & Law, 2010). Although only a proportion of high-risk children will go on to develop an ASD, a much greater number are expected to manifest subclinical ASD-like atypicalities (Ozonoff, Rogers, Farnham & Pennington, 1993; Rogers, 2009), including language difficulties (Piven, Palmer, Landa, Santangelo, Jacobi & Childress, 1997). The few studies that have examined language development in high-risk children show that they are slower to acquire language (Toth, Dawson, Meltzoff, Greenson & Fein, 2007; Yirmiya, Gamliel, Pilowsky, Feldman, Baron-Cohen & Sigman, 2006; Yirmiya, Gamliel, Shaked & Sigman, 2007). There are, however, few studies that have investigated WORD-LEARNING strategies in this population. We know that high-risk three-year-olds have difficulties using ostensive and referential cues to learn a new word–object mapping (Gliga et al., in press), but little is known about available strategies earlier in development. Studying two-year-olds at high BEDFORD ET AL. 32risk for ASD enables us to investigate whether the linguistic difficulties measured in this population are related (1) to difficulties with referent selection through ME, apparent earlier in development (but not later in life; Preissler & Carey, 2005) or (2) to difficulties using feedback for long-term word retention. We first assessed children’s ability to fast map a novel word to an unfamiliar object. Following each trial choice, we either provided no feedback, or ostensively labelled the novel object, thus correcting or reinforcing the child’s initial choice. Word knowledge was retested after a 5-minute break. METHODS Ethical approval was given by NHS NRES London REC (08/H0718/76) and parents gave informed consent. Participants Thirty-one toddlers at high risk for ASD and forty-four low-risk children took part in this study. Six additional children (one low-risk, five high-risk) participated but were not included in the analysis due to non-compliance. To take account of the non-verbal IQ differences (t(71)=3 . 28, p=0 . 002), data for thirteen low-risk toddlers were removed, including two children who had no Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL; Mullen, 1995) data and the eleven children with the highest non-verbal scores. Following exclusion, MSEL non-verbal scores did not differ significantly between the groups (t(52. 5)=1. 68, p=. 1). Participants included in the final analysis were thirty-one low-risk toddlers (13 boys and 18 girls, mean age=24. 3 months, SD=0. 59) and thirty-one high-risk toddlers (14 boys and 17 girls, mean age=24. 6 months, SD=1 . 02). All toddlers were participating in a longitudinal study, the British Autism Study of Infant Siblings (BASIS). Exclusion criteria at intake for both groups included medical or neurological conditions and sensory or motor problems. Children were considered at high risk for ASD by virtue of having an older brother or sister (proband) with a community clinical diagnosis of ASD. Twenty-six probands were male, and five were female. Diagnosis of the proband was confirmed by two expert clinicians (TC, PB) using the Development and Wellbeing Assessment (DAWBA; Goodman, Ford, Richards, Gatward & Meltzer, 2000) and the parent-report Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; (Rutter, Bailey & Lord, 2003). Most probands met criteria for ASD on both the DAWBA and SCQ (n=27). While one proband scored below threshold on the SCQ, no exclusions were made, due to meeting threshold on the DAWBA and expert opinion. For three probands, data were only available for either the DAWBA (n=1) or FEEDBACK REQUIRED FOR VOCABULARY LEARNING 33the SCQ (n=2). For one proband, neither measure was available (aside from parent-confirmed local clinical ASD diagnosis at intake). Parentreported family medical histories were examined for significant medical conditions in the proband or extended families members, with no exclusions made on this basis. The DAWBA is a parent-completed, Web-based questionnaire that combines symptom ratings and narrative description that is then reviewed by an expert clinician. It was used to establish the prevalence of pervasive developmental disorders (ASD) in the UK national children and adolescent mental health survey (Fombonne, 2003). The SCQ is a parent-completed questionnaire with questions developed from the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; Lord, Rutter & LeCouteur, 1994). Children in the low-risk group were recruited from a volunteer database at Birkbeck, Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development. All low-risk infants had at least one older sibling; eighteen male and thirteen female. Screening for possible ASD in these older siblings was undertaken using the SCQ, with all children scoring below the instrument cut-off for ASD (f15). Stimuli Stimuli for the word-learning task were sixteen familiar objects, eight of which were designated ‘ target familiar’ (spoon, toy duck, key, toy horse, ball, toy car, baby shoe, toy pig) and eight of which were designated ‘non-target familiar’ (toy cow, cup, toothbrush, pen, hairbrush, book, fork, comb). An additional eight novel objects were similarly either ‘ target novel’ or ‘non-target novel’ (egg poacher, bottle stopper, lemon juicer, avocado slicer, bottle opener, cooking brush, fried egg shaper, whisk). The ‘ target familiar’ objects were chosen based on the normative MacArthur Bates Communicative Development Inventory (CDI) estimates for two-year-olds (Dale & Fenson, 1996) with all object labels reported to be known by at least 76% of toddlers at that age. For the ‘ target novel’ objects, four novel, bisyllabic, pseudo-words were used: moxi, fimit, kela and togo. The twenty-four objects available were then split into groups of three for each trial, with two familiar objects, and one novel object. Each child completed eight trials, four with ‘ target familiar’ items (Familiar trials) and four with a ‘ target novel’ item (Novel trials). In the Familiar trials, a ‘ target familiar’ object was paired with another ‘non-target familiar’ object and a ‘non-target novel’ object. In the Novel trials, the ‘ target novel’ object was paired with two ‘non-target familiar’ items. Care was taken to ensure that none of the ‘non-target familiar’ objects names were phonologically close to the name used for the novel object. Two alternative options for the assignment of groups of objects to the trial types were created and use of one or the other option was counterbalanced across children in the BEDFORD ET AL. 34two groups. The objects were presented to children on an unpartitioned rectangular tray. Procedure and design The study was split into three phases: FAMILIARIZATION with the objects, FAST-MAPPING and, following a 5-minute delay, RETENTION (see Figure 1). During the initial familiarization period, children played with all of the objects for 5 minutes to ensure that novelty preference would not interfere with children’s later choices in the experimental task (Mather & Plunkett, 2010). An experimenter made sure that the child saw all of the objects by asking the child to place the objects one by one into a box. No objects were named, during this phase, by either the experimenter or the parent. The experimental task then began, with the child seated at a small table, either alone or on the parent’s lap, facing the experimenter. 8 × Fast-mapping (4 × Familiar) (2 × No-feedback) (2 × Feedback) 4 × Retention Where is the shoe? Where is the MOXI? ‘thank you’ ‘thank you’ ‘this is the MOXI’ Where is the MOXI? (4 × Novel) Fig. 1. Experimental paradigm. Example of objects and words used in the fast-mapping and retention trials. FEEDBACK REQUIRED FOR VOCABULARY LEARNING 35Fast-mapping trials. At the start of each trial the experimenter held the tray of objects and, looking at the child, said ‘‘Can you see the spoon/ moxi ?’’ The tray was then placed in front of the child and the experimenter asked ‘‘Can you give me the spoon/moxi?’’ On all Familiar trials and on two of the Novel trials the experimenter responded ‘‘Thank you’’, irrespective of which object the child chose. These were therefore ‘No-feedback’ trials. In the remaining two Novel trials (i.e. Feedback trials) the experimenter ostensively labelled the correct object, holding it in front of the child and responding either ‘‘Yes, this is the moxi. What a nice moxi!’’ or ‘‘No, this is the moxi. What a nice moxi’’ depending on whether the child’s choice had been correct or incorrect. Retention trials. Following a 5-minute break during which children played in the testing room with other toys, there were four retention trials. Pairs of only Novel objects were presented, each including a ‘ target novel’ object and a ‘non-target novel’ object (which had been seen during familiarization and the Familiar fast-mapping trials, but which had never been named). Previously target novel objects were paired with non-targets so that we can test memory for all four target objects, independently. For two of the retention trials, the ‘ target novel’ object had previously been ostensively labelled during fast mapping (Feedback trials), while the other two trials had included no labelling (No-feedback trials). Retention trials again followed one of two predetermined orders, with selection counterbalanced across children. Data analysis Children’s responses were video coded during both the fast-mapping and retention trials. If the child made no response (i.e. did not touch or give any of the objects), then the trial was discarded as invalid. Valid trials were coded as correct or incorrect on the basis of the object given by the child to the examiner (or, if no object was given, then on the basis of the first object touched by the child). A second coder rated trials for four high-risk (13%) and four low-risk (13%) toddlers, yielding 100% agreement between coders. Measures of language and general development General developmental level was assessed at the same visit, using the MSEL (low-risk n=31, high-risk n=31). A non-verbal composite score was calculated as the average of the Visual Reception and Fine Motor scale T-scores. The CDI (Fenson et al., 1993), a parent report measure of vocabulary, was also collected for toddlers in both groups (low-risk n=30, high-risk n=26). A receptive vocabulary count was calculated by combining BEDFORD ET AL. 36the total numbers of words ‘understood’ and words ‘understood and said’ for each child. Group characteristics are shown in Table 1. RESULTS We analyze separately the fast-mapping and the retention trials. In each case, we used a mixed-factorial ANOVA to test for differences in word learning performance as a result of Group (varying between-subjects; lowrisk, high-risk), and fast-mapping Item type (varying within-subjects; Novel, Familiar) or retention Feedback type (also varying within-subjects; Feedback, No-feedback). The number of valid trials did not differ significantly between Groups (Novel item: High-Risk M=3 . 9, Low-Risk M=4. 0; Familiar item: High-Risk M=3. 9, Low-Risk M=4. 0; Ostensive feedback: High-Risk M=1 . 9, Low-Risk M=1 . 9; No-feedback: High-Risk M=2. 0, Low-Risk M=1. 9). We also compare performance to chance levels to highlight for which group and under which conditions participants successfully choose the correct referent or remembered its label. We subsequently reanalyzed retention data by separating those trials for which ostensive feedback provided either a correction of an initially incorrect choice or reinforcement of an initially correct choice.1 Fast-mapping One sample t-tests against a chance level of 0. 33 showed that both low-risk and high-risk toddlers performed significantly above chance in selecting the correct object in both Novel and Familiar fast-mapping trials (low-risk TABLE 1. Group descriptives Low-risk (n=31) High-risk (n=31) Age 24. 3 (0. 59) 24. 6 (1. 0) F:M 18:13 17:14 CDI (n=30) (n=26) Receptive vocabulary count 449. 0 (172. 1) 335. 2 (166. 8)* Mullen (n=31) (n=31) Non-verbal ability (T-score) 53. 58 (7. 03) 49. 79 (10. 45) Verbal ability (T-score) 57. 55 (7. 74) 50. 12 (12. 39)* NOTE: * indicates that scores are significantly different at the p
Posted on: Thu, 04 Jul 2013 07:55:06 +0000

Trending Topics



Recently Viewed Topics




© 2015